BaYaka camp in Congo. Image courtesy of Nikhil Chaudhary

Hunter-gatherers can help us understand the conditions that children may be psychologically adapted to because we lived as hunter-gatherers for 95% of our evolutionary history. Paying greater attention to hunter-gatherer childhoods may help economically developed countries improve education and wellbeing.

Parents now have much less childcare support from their familial and social networks than would likely have been the case during most of our evolutionary history

Nikhil Chaudhary

探花直播benefits of skin-to-skin contact for both parents and infants are already recognised, but other behaviours common in hunter-gatherer societies may also benefit families in economically developed countries, a Cambridge researcher suggests.

Parents and children may, for instance, benefit from a larger network of people being involved in care-giving, as seen in hunter-gatherer societies. Increasing staff-to-child ratios in nurseries to bring them closer to highly attentive hunter-gatherer ratios could support learning and wellbeing. And more peer-to-peer, active and mixed-age learning, as seen in hunter-gatherer communities, may help school children in developed countries.

Published today in the Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, the听study by , and Dr Annie Swanepoel, a child psychiatrist, calls for new research into child mental health in hunter-gatherer societies. They explore the possibility that some common aspects of hunter-gatherer childhoods could help families in economically developed countries. Eventually, hunter-gatherer behaviours could inform 鈥榚xperimental intervention trials鈥 in homes, schools and nurseries.

探花直播authors acknowledge that children living in hunter-gatherer societies live in very different environments and circumstances than those in developed countries. They also stress that hunter-gatherer children invariably face many difficulties that are not experienced in developed countries and, therefore, caution that these childhoods should not be idealised.

Drawing on his own observations of the BaYaka people in Congo and the extensive research of anthropologists studying other hunter-gatherer societies, Dr Chaudhary highlights major differences in the ways in which hunter-gatherer children are cared for compared to their peers in developed countries. He stresses that 鈥渃ontemporary hunter-gatherers must not be thought of as 鈥榣iving fossils鈥, and while their ways of life may offer some clues about our prehistory, they are still very much modern populations each with a unique cultural and demographic history鈥.听

Physical contact and attentiveness

Despite increasing uptake of baby carriers and baby massage in developed countries, levels of physical contact with infants remain far higher in hunter-gatherer societies. In Botswana, for instance, 10-20 week old !Kung infants are in physical contact with someone for around 90% of daylight hours, and almost 100% of crying bouts are responded to, almost always with comforting or nursing 鈥 scolding is extremely rare.

探花直播study points out that this exceptionally attentive childcare is made possible because of the major role played by non-parental caregivers, or 鈥榓lloparents鈥, which is far rarer in developed countries.

Non-parental caregivers

In many hunter-gatherer societies, alloparents provide almost half of a child鈥檚 care. A previous study found that in the DRC, Efe infants have 14 alloparents a day by the time they are 18 weeks old, and are passed between caregivers eight times an hour.

Dr Chaudhary said: 鈥淧arents now have much less childcare support from their familial and social networks than would likely have been the case during most of our evolutionary history. Such differences seem likely to create the kind of evolutionary mismatches that could be harmful to both caregivers and children.鈥

鈥 探花直播availability of other caregivers can reduce the negative impacts of stress within the nuclear family, and the risk of maternal depression, which has knock-on effects for child wellbeing and cognitive development.鈥

探花直播study emphasises that alloparenting is a core human adaptation, contradicting 鈥榠ntensive mothering鈥 narratives which emphasise that mothers should use their maternal instincts to manage childcare alone. Dr Chaudhary and Dr Swanepoel write that 鈥榮uch narratives can听lead to maternal exhaustion and听have dangerous consequences鈥.

Care-giving ratios

探花直播study points out that communal living in hunter-gatherer societies results in a very high ratio听of available听caregivers to听infants/toddlers,听which can even exceed 10:1.

This听contrasts starkly听with听the nuclear family unit, and even more so听with听nursery settings, in developed countries. According to the UK鈥檚 Department of Education regulations, nurseries听require听ratios of 1 carer to 3听children aged under 2 years, or 1 carer to 4 children aged 2-3.

Dr Chaudhary said: 鈥淎lmost all day, hunter-gatherer infants and toddlers have a capable caregiver within a couple of metres of them. From the infant鈥檚 perspective, that proximity and responsiveness, is very different from what is experienced in many nursery settings in the UK.鈥

鈥淚f that ratio is stretched even thinner, we need to consider the possibility that this could have impacts on children's wellbeing.鈥

Children providing care and mixed-age active learning

In hunter-gatherer societies, children play a significantly bigger role in providing care to infants and toddlers than is the case in developed countries. In some communities they begin providing some childcare from the age of four and are capable of sensitive caregiving; and it is common to see older, but still pre-adolescent children looking after infants.

By contrast, the NSPCC in the UK recommends that when leaving pre-adolescent children at home, babysitters should be in their late teens at least.

Dr Chaudhary said: 鈥淚n developed countries, children are busy with schooling and may have less opportunity to develop caregiving competence. However, we should at least explore the possibility that older siblings could play a greater role in supporting their parents, which might also enhance their own social development.鈥

探花直播study also points out that instructive teaching is rare in hunter-gatherer societies and that infants primarily learn via observation and imitation. From around the age of two, hunter-gatherer children spend large portions of the day in mixed-age (2-16) 鈥榩laygroups鈥 without adult supervision. There, they learn from one another, acquiring skills and knowledge collaboratively via highly active play practice and exploration.

Learning and play are two sides of the same coin, which contrasts with the lesson-time / play-time dichotomy of schooling in the UK and other developed countries.

Dr Chaudhary and Dr Swanepoel note that 鈥淐lassroom schooling is often at odds with the modes of learning typical of human evolutionary history.鈥 探花直播study acknowledges that children living in hunter-gatherer societies live in very different environments and circumstances than those in developed countries:

鈥淔oraging skills are very different to those required to make a living in market-economies, and classroom teaching is certainly necessary to learn the latter. But children may possess certain psychological learning adaptations that can be practically harnessed in some aspects of their schooling. When peer and active learning can be incorporated, they have been shown to improve motivation and performance, and reduce stress.鈥 探花直播authors also highlight that physical activity interventions听have been shown to aid performance among students diagnosed with ADHD.听

Further research

探花直播study calls for more research into children鈥檚 mental health in hunter-gatherer societies to test whether the hypothesised evolutionary mismatches actually exist. If they do, such insights could then be used to direct experimental intervention trials in developed countries.

Working with a team from the Royal College of Psychiatrists, Dr Chaudhary and Dr Swanepoel hope that greater collaboration between evolutionary anthropologists and child psychiatrists/psychologists can help to advance our understanding of the conditions that children need to thrive.

Reference

N听Chaudhary听and听A听Swanepoel, 鈥樷, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry (2023). DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.13773



探花直播text in this work is licensed under a . Images, including our videos, are Copyright 漏 探花直播 of Cambridge and licensors/contributors as identified.听 All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways 鈥 as here, on our main website under its Terms and conditions, and on a range of channels including social media that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.